Lately my 6-year-old has been difficult about his piano
practice. His new trick goes like
this: I say something along the lines of,
“Okay, why don’t you start with The Wild
Horseman today?”
He shoots me a rebellious look and replies. “Fine, I’ll play
The Wild Horseman”. He puts his hands in the correct starting
position, and proceeds to sing the
entire song, note perfect, while moving his fingers over the correct keys, but
not pressing them down. He then grins saucily at
me.
I put on my “mom” voice.
“Very funny. Now play it
properly, with your fingers, not your voice.”
What I don’t tell him is that this is actually not a half-bad way of
practicing.
Practice forms mental
representations of the music
When we practice music what we’re actually doing is forming
a mental representation of the song.
When we play it for the first time, we read it note-by-note (or maybe
chord-by-chord, if we’re more experienced), but as we practice, we stop having
to focus on the individual notes, and instead they become encoded in our brain
as a whole sequence of notes. Once we have practiced the song enough, we just
have to start it, and the notes follow one another, like beads on a
string. This is true for both the movements we make while playing and the sounds
that are produced, because we form both a motor representation of the song AND
an auditory representation of the song.
That is, we learn the movements we need to make, and we learn what the
song sounds like. These two
representations are closely tied together in our brains and they support each
other. Scientists have a special name
(don’t they always?) for this connection between the movements we make and the
sensations that are produced: “sensorimotor integration”.
Imagine playing your favourite piece of music on an
electronic keyboard. Now imagine playing
it with the keyboard turned off, so there is no sound. It would be much harder, wouldn’t it? We need that auditory feedback to help keep
our motor program running properly. In
fact, the best way to hit all the right notes on the soundless keyboard is to
play a mental soundtrack of the song while performing the movements. This works because the parts of our brain
that store and produce the motor pattern are intricately linked to the parts of
our brain that listen to the sounds we produce by playing. And these auditory parts of the brain are activated during mental imagery of music.
The connection between movement and sound works the other
way around too. It’s been shown that if
you are listening to a piece of music that you know how to play, motor parts of
your brain are activated, as if you were playing along.
Sensorimotor
integration aids musical memory
A recent paper from McGill
University explores the role of sensorimotor
integration in musical memory. The
researchers, Rachel Brown and Caroline Palmer, had pianists learn short
melodies in one of four different ways:
1) by simply listening to them, 2) by practicing the songs on a soundless keyboard 3) by practicing them on a keyboard with sound or 4) by practicing them along with
recorded version of the songs, but unable to hear their own playing. The pianists were then tested
to see whether they recognized the melodies from among a pool of other melodies
they had to listen to. Pianists were also
tested to see how good their auditory and motor imagery was.
The researchers found that practicing without any auditory
feedback (i.e. on a soundless keyboard) made it quite hard to recognize the
melodies after. It was much worse than
normal practicing (which was the best), practicing with a recording (2nd
best), or just listening to the tunes (3rd best). However, pianists with good auditory imagery were the most successful at recognizing melodies they had practiced without sound. In other
words, if the pianists were better at mentally “singing along” with their
soundless practice, they were better able to recognize those tunes later.
Direct auditory feedback makes for the strongest sensorimotor associations
Another interesting result from this paper
came from comparing practice where the movements and sounds were either
“strongly coupled” or “weakly coupled”.
Strongly coupled meant that the pianists could hear their own playing,
so there was a complete and direct connection between the movements the
pianists made and the sounds they heard.
In weakly coupled practice, the pianists could not hear their own
playing, but they heard a recorded version of the melody. What this meant was that as long as they
played exactly correctly (in terms of both pitch and rhythm), the sounds they
heard were connected to the movements they made. But if they hit the wrong key on the keyboard
or were a little slow in their rhythms, this was not reflected in the sounds
they heard. What the researchers found
was that strongly coupled practice made for stronger memories of the melodies
than weakly coupled practice. The
conclusion was that direct feedback of the effects of the movements seemed to
be required for the strongest auditory-motor associations.
My son, while intending to be silly, is practicing his mental representation of the melody
by singing it. And moving his fingers at
the same time practices his representation of the motor task of playing the song. What’s lacking is the direct
feedback: if he makes a mistake with his fingers, it won’t result in a wrong
note in his singing. So if he makes a
lot of mistakes, this isn’t going to help his motor representation. But since in this case he’s playing a song that he
actually knows quite well, it’s not a terrible way to practice (and certainly
better than not practicing at all!)
Another way to think about this type of “practicing” is that
it’s a good way to warm up the brain for the physical practicing of this song. In fact, a really good warm-up might just be
to sit and look over the music and imagine playing it, thinking about how the
hands would move and what the song would sound like. This is mental practicing… but I think that’s
a topic for another post.
Reference:
Brown RM, Palmer C. Auditory–motor learning influences
auditory memory for music. Memory & Cognition. 2012. Available at:
http://www.springerlink.com/index/10.3758/s13421-011-0177-x. Accessed April 23, 2012.
This is what we're finding with HearFones, that give singers 'strongly coupled' feedback of the sounds they're making at their mouth. The new higher-frequency "details" they hear seem to immediately cause them to correct and eliminate them, and consequently to internalize these new techniques. I really look forward to reading your reference paper.
ReplyDeleteWe've also heard your 6-year-old effect: put HearFones on the 10 kids in a childrens choir and the "kids singing" sound goes away. We get 10 young voices beautifully singing on pitch and with lovely voices. Amazing!