Christine, the lovely parish co-ordinator
at our local church, smiles at me across the desk. I’m there to pay the rental charge for my
piano studio, located in the church basement.
But she has something else in mind.
“Are you around this Christmas?” She raises
her eyebrows hopefully. “Are you
available to play at the Christmas Eve service?”
Last year we were away, traveling at
Christmas, but in previous years I’ve played my flute at the church on
Christmas Eve, adding some shimmer to an otherwise quiet evening for the small
congregation. Two years ago I also roped
my children into singing, to the delight of the elderly ladies celebrating
their Christmas worship. This year we
don’t have any plans for Christmas Eve, so I willingly agree to perform
something. I know I’ll need to plan
music for a prelude, postlude, offertory and a solo during communion, in
addition to adding a flute descant to the hymns.
What’s left to decide is the hard
part: what to play. I’ll wait for the church music committee to
tell me what the hymns will be, and then I can pick my incidental music from
the remainder of the canon of Christmas tunes.
I have to confess, I am not thrilled about
any of those tunes. I’ve heard and
played them all a million times before.
As a child, I loved Christmas music. I started early every year, spending a large
chunk of the fall belting out Christmas songs, often in the bathroom where I mistakenly
felt no one could hear me. My very first
book of sheet music, given to me before I could read music, and well before I
started playing an instrument, was a collection of Christmas carols. I remember poring over the words, memorizing
every verse. Later, after I started
studying piano, the music itself was learned and played endlessly. The book, now battered and torn, still gets
pulled out every year.
But after forty-some Decembers of listening
to the same Christmas songs, I admit to being entirely bored by them. Silent
Night? No thanks. O Come
All Ye Faithful? Snore…. Even the slightly less common carols seem
utterly mundane. However, I still have
to pick at least four to play on Christmas Eve.
There are
newer, less familiar carols. The problem
is that no one really wants to hear them on Christmas Eve. It turns out that it’s a natural human
phenomenon to prefer tunes we already know.
Scientists call it the Mere Exposure
Effect: simply being exposed to
something (in this case a tune) makes you like it more when you meet it a
second time. The Mere Exposure Effect in
music was first shown over one hundred years ago, and holds true for music of
all styles. Research studies that look
at this effect generally work like this:
volunteers listen to a series of (usually unfamiliar) melodies. Then, later, they hear another series of
melodies that includes some of the melodies they have heard before, and some
new melodies. In this second listening
period, they have to rate each tune on how much they like it. The results show that hearing a piece of
music (even only once!) predisposes you to like it more than other music you
have not heard before.
The Mere Exposure Effect tells us something
about the different ways in which we remember music. And that’s because this effect holds true
whether you remember having heard the tune before or not. If people are distracted with another task
while hearing the music, they often don’t remember they’ve heard it before, but
they will still give it a higher rating than tunes they’ve never heard. This shows that the mere exposure effect is
dependent on our implicit, unconscious memory for music. In contrast, if we consciously remember
having heard the tune before, this uses our explicit memory. So we might not
remember having heard an unfamiliar Christmas carol before, but the more we’ve
heard it, the more we’ll like it.
Researchers want to understand why this
effect works the way it does: what is it
about being exposed to something that makes us enjoy it more? One theory is the perceptual fluency model, which proposes that when we’ve been
exposed to something before, our brains have an easier time processing the
information. The first time we hear a
new tune, we don’t know exactly what will come next, and so, whether we’re aware
of it or not, our minds are constantly trying to guess what the next note or
phrase will be. That’s a lot of work,
and makes us feel as if the tune is difficult.
When we’ve heard the music before, we might not remember exactly how it
goes, but our brain does a better job of predicting the next note, and this is
less work, making us like the music more.
A second theory is called the two-factor model. This idea is that our aesthetic rating of a
piece of music is based on two things:
our dislike for things that are too new or complex, and our boredom with
things that are two simple or familiar.
In other words, we like to be able to predict what will happen next, but
if it is completely obvious what will happen next, that’s no fun. The two-factor model suggests that in
addition to the mere exposure effect, there should also be a satiation effect,
in which people start to dislike stimuli that they have been exposed to too
many times. (As in... ahem… Christmas
carols).
|
Studies looking for a satiation effect have
had mixed results, depending on the stimuli used, but it looks like the more
realistic the stimuli are, the more likely they are to generate a satiation
effect. This was found for musical
stimuli in a study from the University of Toronto (Szpunar et al., 2004). When the researchers in this study played
computer generated atonal sequences for people, they did not find that the
subjects got bored of them, even after 64 exposures. However, when the same experiment was
conducted using excerpts of orchestral recordings, they found that people gave
lower ratings to the excerpts they had heard many times.
Where does this leave me in my Christmas
Eve music planning? Well, clearly I want
to try to hit that sweet spot in the liking curve, by playing something that
people have heard before, but not too many times. I’ll aim for less-familiar carols, and maybe
include something that most people won’t have heard. They may not appreciate this year, but when
they hear that tune again another Christmas, they’ll like it better.
References
Green, A.C., Bærentsen, K.B.,
Stødkilde-Jørgensen, H., Roepstorff, A., and Vuust, P. (2012). Listen, learn,
like! Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex involved in the mere exposure effect in
music. Neurol Res Int 2012, 846270.
Peretz,
I., Gaudreau, D., and Bonnel, A.-M. (1998). Exposure effects on music
preference and recognition. Memory & Cognition 26, 884–902.
Szpunar, K.K., Schellenberg,
E.G., and Pliner, P. (2004). Liking and Memory for Musical Stimuli as a
Function of Exposure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Cognition 30, 370–381.
No comments:
Post a Comment